تعداد نشریات | 5 |
تعداد شمارهها | 111 |
تعداد مقالات | 1,247 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 1,199,583 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 1,060,300 |
Syntactic Priming: Is Shared Semantic Content an Obligation? | ||
Journal of Applied Linguistics and Applied Literature: Dynamics and Advances | ||
مقاله 4، دوره 12، شماره 1 - شماره پیاپی 23، تیر 2024، صفحه 51-62 اصل مقاله (613.15 K) | ||
نوع مقاله: Research Article | ||
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): 10.22049/jalda.2024.28639.1547 | ||
نویسندگان | ||
Ali Akbar Ansarin1؛ Masoud Yaghoubi Notash2؛ shalaleh Javadi* 3 | ||
1Professor of Applied Linguistics, Department of English language and Literature, Faculty of Persian Literature and Foreign Languages, University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran | ||
2Associate Professor of Applied Linguistics, Department of English language and Literature, Faculty of Persian Literature and Foreign Language, University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran | ||
3PhD Candidate, University of Tabriz, Tabriz, Iran | ||
چکیده | ||
Syntactic priming has been suggested to be an efficient paradigm in studying mental language representations. However, further research is needed to explore the underlying mechanisms. Recently it is suggested that argument-based constructions are present at both the syntactic and discourse levels of representation predicting that priming effect does not occur in the absence of shared semantic content. The study used a pre-test and post-test approach within a quasi-experimental design to investigate whether sentences with no shared semantic content, but similar syntactic structure, could prime one another in L2 written production tasks. Ninety students at the University of Tabriz participated in the study and were divided into intermediate or upper-intermediate groups based on their proficiency test performance. Both groups narrated a silent movie in the pre-treatment phase. In the treatment phase, the participants were primed with motion phrasal verbs by reading and rating a booklet including pictures followed by phrasal motion verbs describing them. Immediately afterward, they were required to narrate a silent movie. It was hypothesized that if semantically unrelated structures could prime one another as is supported by some reported findings, priming participants with motion phrasal verbs would boost non-motion phrasal verb usage in the treatment phase. However, the authors failed to find a significant difference between the performance of participants in the pre-treatment vs. post-treatment phase. The findings support the claim that syntactic similarity is not sufficient to trigger structural priming, and shared semantics seems to be required, and are justified with regard to semantic roles and compositional vs. non-compositional meaning. | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
Structural Priming؛ Syntax؛ Semantics؛ L2 production؛ Semantic roles | ||
مراجع | ||
Bannard, C., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Modeling children’s early grammatical knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(41), 17284–17289. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0905638106
Bidgood, A., Pine, J. M., Rowland, C. F., & Ambridge, B. (2020). Syntactic Representations Are Both Abstract and Semantically Constrained: Evidence from Children’s and Adults’ Comprehension and Production/Priming of the English Passive. Cognitive Science, 44(9). https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12892
Bock, J. K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6
Bock, K. (1990). Framing sentences. Cognition, 35(1), 1–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90035-i
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 177–192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.177
Branigan, H. P., & Pickering, M. J. (2017). An experimental approach to linguistic representation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x16002028
Chen, X., Hartsuiker, R. J., Muylle, M., Slim, M. S., & Zhang, C. (2022). The effect of animacy on structural Priming: A replication of Bock, Loebell and Morey (1992). Journal of Memory and Language, 127, 104354.
Chomsky, N. (1994) Bare phrase structure. Cambridge, MA: Distributed by MIT Working Papers in Linguistics.
Chomsky, N. (1995). Language and Nature. Mind, 104(413), 1–61. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/104.413.1
Eddington, D. T., De Mendoza, F. J. R., De Knop, S., Boers, F., & De Rycker, A. (2010). Argument constructions and language processing: Evidence from a priming Experiment and pedagogical implications. In De Gruyter eBooks (pp. 213–238). https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110245837.213
Fedorenko, E., Blank, I. A., Siegelman, M., & Mineroff, Z. (2020). Lack of selectivity for syntax relative to word meanings throughout the language network. Cognition, 203, 104348.
Fillmore, C. J. (1968). Lexical entries for verbs. Foundations of language, 373-393.
Fillmore, C. J. (1977). The case for case reopened. In Grammatical relations. Brill.
Goldberg, Adele E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (1996). Words by Default: Optimizing Constraints and the Persian Complex Predicate. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 22(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v22i1.1360
Goldberg, A. E. (2003). Constructions: a new theoretical approach to language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(5), 219–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(03)00080-9
Hare, M., & Goldberg, A. E. (1999). Structural priming: Purely syntactic? In Psychology Press eBooks (pp. 208–211). https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410603494-41
Kaschak, M. P., Kutta, T. J., & Jones, J. R. (2011). Structural priming as implicit learning: Cumulative priming effects and individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1133–1139. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0157-y
Kidd, E. (2012). Implicit statistical learning is directly associated with the acquisition of syntax. Developmental Psychology, 48(1), 171–184. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025405
Lakoff, G. (1970). Linguistics and natural logic. Synthese, 22(1–2), 151–271. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00413602
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: from intention to articulation. Choice Reviews Online, 27(04), 27–1947. https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.27-1947
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125(1), 49–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.008
Saffran, J. R. (2002). Constraints on Statistical Language Learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 47(1), 172–196. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2839
Segalowitz, N. (2008). Automaticity and Second Languages. In Blackwell Publishing Ltd eBooks (pp. 382–408). https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756492.ch13
Shin, J. E. N., & Christianson, K. (2012). Structural Priming and Second Language Learning. Language Learning, 62(3), 931–964. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00657.x
Ziegler, J., Bencini, G., Goldberg, A., & Snedeker, J. (2019). How abstract is syntax? Evidence from structural priming. Cognition, 193, 104045. https://doi.org/101016/j.cognition.2019.104045 | ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 237 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 216 |